In general, the studies performed to date on the Immediate Feedback Assessment
Technique (the “IF-AT”) show that the answer-until-correct procedure actively
engages learners in the correction of initially inaccurate responses, plays a
crucial role in the acquisition of information, aides incorporation of accurate
information into cognitive processes, and permits retrieval of correct answers
during retention tests. The studies have further shown that students evaluated
with Scantron forms typically have significantly lower retention and repeat more
initial mistakes on subsequent examinations than students tested with the
answer-until-correct method (i.e., the IF-AT).
The effectiveness of the IF-AT has been demonstrated in a number of studies
conducted in classrooms and laboratories, with enhancements in student
performance observed during cumulative assessments. In these studies, the
provision of corrective information during classroom and laboratory tests
reduced the likelihood of incorrect repeated responses on subsequent item
administrations presented in their original or with a modified wording. The
correction of initially inaccurate responses was maintained across retention
intervals of 2 to 10 weeks and across populations ranging from preschool
children with developmental delays to university students preparing for
classroom examinations. Similar gains were not observed when responses were
recorded with Scantron forms, and only intermediate gains were observed when
feedback was provided either after the completion of a test or after a 24-hour
delay.
Students without known learning difficulties (elementary school students through
college students) reported that immediate feedback and the opportunity to
respond until correct provided realistic assessments of performance, more
involvement in the testing process, and the opportunity to exit an item with the
correct solution. Students with diagnosed learning difficulties reported that
immediate feedback enhanced the acquisition of basic academic materials (e.g.,
colors, shapes), enhanced rote memorization once a concept and the function of
that concept were grasped (e.g., learning multiplication skills in discrete
units), and that the answer-until-correct procedure enabled the completion of
each test item as a discrete unit that could be resolved before approaching the
next test item.
Among the most common advantages of the IF-AT reported by educators with special
needs children in their classrooms and resource rooms was that the IF-AT not
only provided feedback, but that the answer-until-correct procedure prompted
students to continue to respond until the correct solution was attained. This
latter advantage was not achieved when feedback was provided by an educator, as
students required continuous prompting to maintain responding. When the IF-AT
was used, feedback coupled to the opportunity to answer-until-correct could be
provided to an entire class, whereas an educator could provide this combination
to only one student at a time, which suggests a tremendous potential for the
IF-AT as an adjunctive tool with special-needs learners under appropriate
classroom conditions. For example, the IF-AT might not be appropriate for
teaching concepts and their applications and functions, but it would be highly
effective for assisting learners during rote memorization drills, such as those
used while rehearsing mathematical fact series. The concept and function of an
arithmetic operation, in keeping with NCTM 2000 principles and standards
(National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, 2000), must be presented by an
educator. However, the encouragement of active learning, the provision of
immediate feedback, and the use of an answer-until-correct procedure that
maximizes time on task, are three cardinal principles of pedagogy (Rickey,
1995), and they are easily employed with the IF-AT.
|